What Is the Other-Race Effect in Psychology?

The “other-race effect” (ORE) is a cognitive phenomenon where individuals find it more difficult to recognize faces from racial groups different from their own. Also known as the “cross-race effect” or “own-race bias,” this widely documented psychological effect highlights a systematic difference in how people process and remember faces based on racial group membership.

Understanding the Other-Race Effect

The other-race effect often leads to the perception that individuals from another racial group “all look alike.” This cognitive bias means people may struggle to distinguish between individuals of a different race, even if they have seen those faces before. Studies consistently show that people are more accurate at recognizing faces from their own racial group.

This phenomenon is not linked to prejudice or discriminatory attitudes; it is a cognitive bias rooted in how the brain processes visual information. The ORE begins to develop in infancy. While newborns show no racial preferences, by three months of age, infants primarily exposed to own-race individuals start to exhibit this effect. It becomes more pronounced as individuals age, shaped by early visual and sociocultural experiences.

Explaining the Other-Race Effect

Two primary psychological theories attempt to explain the mechanisms behind the other-race effect: the perceptual expertise hypothesis and social cognitive theories.

Perceptual Expertise Hypothesis

The perceptual expertise hypothesis suggests that individuals develop more refined perceptual skills for faces within their own racial group due to increased experience. This frequent exposure leads to more detailed encoding of own-race faces. Evidence supporting this theory comes from developmental research, showing that the ORE increases as children gain more experience with own-race faces.

Social Cognitive Theories

Social cognitive theories propose that social categorization and group membership influence face processing. People tend to process “in-group” faces, those belonging to their own social group, more individually, focusing on unique features. Conversely, “out-group” faces are often processed more categorically, with less attention paid to distinguishing individual features, leading to a more generalized representation.

Neural correlates provide insights into these processing differences. Brain regions involved in face perception, such as the fusiform face area (FFA), show different activation patterns when viewing own-race versus other-race faces. Difficulties with other-race faces can emerge early in perceptual processing, requiring more effortful processing for successful encoding and retrieval into long-term memory.

Real-World Consequences

The other-race effect has substantial practical implications, particularly in contexts requiring accurate face recognition.

Eyewitness Identification

The ORE significantly impacts the accuracy of eyewitness testimony, especially in cross-racial identifications, which can contribute to wrongful convictions. Studies have shown that an eyewitness is over 50% more likely to make a misidentification when the perpetrator and witness are of different races. For instance, approximately one-third of wrongful convictions overturned by DNA analysis have involved white individuals misidentifying Black individuals. Research indicates that participants are 1.40 times more likely to correctly identify own-race faces but 1.56 times more likely to falsely identify other-race faces.

Implicit Bias and Social Interactions

Beyond the legal system, the ORE can subtly influence social perceptions and interactions. The difficulty in distinguishing between individuals of other racial groups can contribute to implicit biases, making it harder to form distinct impressions in initial encounters. This can lead to “cognitive disregard,” where individuals automatically encode an other-race face without fully processing its unique features. Such recognition failures can contribute to social isolation.

Addressing the Effect

While the other-race effect is a robust phenomenon, it is not fixed and can be influenced by various factors and interventions. Strategies aimed at mitigating the ORE often focus on increasing exposure and encouraging individuated processing of other-race faces.

Increased Intergroup Contact

Increased intergroup contact and diverse social interactions can improve face recognition abilities over time. More frequent and meaningful exposure to individuals of other racial groups can enhance perceptual experience and reduce categorical processing. Research supports that greater interracial contact can reduce the size of the observed ORE, particularly when it encourages individuals to individuate others. Positive experiences with individuals from different racial backgrounds can also reduce anxiety related to cross-race interactions.

Individuation Strategies

Techniques that encourage individuals to focus on the unique features of other-race faces, rather than their group membership, can be effective in reducing the ORE. This approach, known as individuation training, can be incorporated into programs for professions where accurate cross-racial identification is important, such as law enforcement. Studies have shown that training preschool-aged children to individuate other-race faces can reduce implicit racial bias. This type of training helps people differentiate among multiple other-race individuals.

How to Identify the Highlighted Vessel

Key Components of Cardiac Muscle Structure and Function Explained

Can Meditation Help Lower Your Blood Pressure?