What Is a Quasi-Experiment? Key Characteristics and Examples

Researchers aim to understand relationships between variables, often seeking to establish cause and effect. They employ various study designs, each with unique strengths and limitations. Quasi-experiments offer a valuable approach for investigating interventions and outcomes, especially when certain experimental controls are not feasible. This article explains quasi-experiments and provides examples of their application.

Distinguishing Quasi-Experiments

Quasi-experiments differ from true experiments primarily in how participants are assigned to groups. In a true experiment, researchers use random assignment, ensuring every participant has an equal chance of being placed in any study group, such as a treatment or control group. This randomization helps ensure groups are comparable at the outset, minimizing pre-existing differences that could influence results.

In contrast, quasi-experiments do not use random assignment. Participants are assigned to groups based on pre-existing conditions, self-selection, or other non-random criteria. This absence of randomization means the groups being compared may differ in ways beyond the studied intervention. While both true and quasi-experiments examine cause-and-effect relationships, the lack of random assignment in quasi-experiments introduces considerations regarding internal validity.

Key Characteristics of Quasi-Experimental Design

A quasi-experimental design assesses the impact of an intervention or treatment on an outcome, similar to a true experiment. It involves manipulating an independent variable, the factor changed by the researcher. A dependent variable, representing the outcome, is then measured to observe effects.

The defining feature of quasi-experiments is the use of pre-existing groups or the inability to randomly assign participants. Researchers work with groups already formed or where assignment to a condition is determined by factors outside their control. This design is chosen when random assignment is impossible or impractical. Researchers still strive to draw conclusions about causal links, but they must account for the potential influence of confounding variables due to non-random group formation.

Practical Examples of Quasi-Experiments

Quasi-experiments are frequently used in real-world settings where random assignment is not feasible or ethical. One common example involves evaluating the impact of a new educational program. Imagine a school district implementing a new teaching method in only two of its elementary schools, while other schools continue with the traditional curriculum. Researchers could compare student performance (dependent variable) in the schools adopting the new method (treatment group) with those using the old method (comparison group). Randomly assigning students or schools to different methods might be disruptive or impractical, making this a quasi-experiment.

Another instance might be studying the effects of a natural disaster on mental health outcomes. Researchers could compare the psychological well-being (dependent variable) of individuals living in a community directly affected by a hurricane (treatment group) versus those in a similar, unaffected community (comparison group). The “intervention” here, the hurricane, is a naturally occurring event that cannot be manipulated or randomly assigned. This scenario allows for the investigation of real-world impacts that would be impossible to study through controlled experimentation.

A third example could involve assessing the effectiveness of a new public health policy, such as a smoking ban in public places. Researchers might compare rates of respiratory illnesses (dependent variable) in a city that implemented the ban (treatment group) with a demographically similar city that did not (comparison group). The policy implementation acts as the independent variable. Since individuals cannot be randomly assigned to live in cities with or without smoking bans, this research design fits the quasi-experimental framework.

Situations for Using Quasi-Experiments

Researchers often turn to quasi-experimental designs when true experimental designs are impractical, unethical, or impossible to conduct. For example, it would be unethical to randomly assign individuals to groups where one group is exposed to a harmful substance like cigarette smoke, while another is not, to study health effects. In such cases, researchers might use quasi-experiments by comparing the health outcomes of existing smokers with non-smokers.

This design is also valuable for studying the effects of large-scale interventions or natural events that researchers cannot control. Policies, educational reforms, or environmental changes often affect pre-existing groups, making random assignment impossible. While quasi-experiments may have limitations regarding internal validity compared to true experiments, they offer higher external validity because they examine interventions in real-world contexts. They provide a practical way to investigate cause-and-effect relationships in situations where other rigorous designs are not viable.